Introduction

The nature of beauty is a long-standing question in many disciplines. A critical issue from psychologists and neuroscientists is whether there is a common neural basis representing different forms of beauty in the brain. Researchers have attempted to investigate this issue using experimental approaches to assessing aesthetic responses to beauty while brain activities were recorded (Aharon et al., 2001; Chatterjee & Vartanian, 2014; Pearce et al., 2016). Almost two decades have passed. There is still no consensus about whether there is a unique brain representation specific to beauty (Conway & Rehding, 2013; Ishizu & Zeki, 2011; Skov & Nadal, 2020). Inconsistent findings across studies might suggest that there are beautify form-specific modules in the brain. The discrepant results also might reflect variations of tasks and relevant parameters used in these studies, although there might be a common beauty-representation in the brain. One way to examine these two views is to conduct meta-analysis by synthesizing the empirical neuroimaging studies in the field. This was conducted here by using activation likelihood estimation (ALE) meta-analyses, which focused on studies that examined the experience of beauty.Footnote 1

From the computational view of cognition (Marr, 1982/2010), understanding how beauty is experienced in humans may need different level analyses: implementational level, algorithmic-representational level, and computational level. As pointed out by Pearce et al. (2016), brain imaging studies mainly address the implementation level and less on algorithmic-representational or computational levels. Thus, one way to define the common neurobiological implementation of beauty is to test the existence of overlapping brain regions when experiencing the beauty from different sources (e.g., faces or visual art rated as beautiful). This overlap-based approach has been widely used to search the common neural basis of different cognitive processes. For instance, recent studies reported that the right inferior parietal lobule engaged in processing spatial, temporal, and social distance (Parkinson, Liu, & Wheatley, 2014), the left intraparietal sulcus was associated with processing both perceptually salient and socially salient stimuli (Sui, Liu, Mevorach, & Humphreys, 2015), and the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and the left anterior insula were involved in processing both psychological and physical selves (Hu et al., 2016). In this sense, a common neural basis for different forms of beauty can be identified if there are common regions activated by experiencing different forms of beauty. The question remains: are there such common brain regions that convergently are activated with different forms of beauty? There are contradictory arguments to this question.

Some researchers claimed that a brain region specific to beauty exists, regardless of the sources of beauty and task type (Ishizu & Zeki, 2011, 2013; Zeki, Romaya, Benincasa, & Atiyah, 2014). For instance, Ishizu and Zeki (2011) reported that the medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) is activated when experiencing both musical and visual beauty. The follow-up work from the same group showed that viewing mathematical formulas also activated the mOFC (Zeki et al., 2014). Based on these results, Ishizu and Zeki (2011) concluded that there is a single neural characteristic within the mOFC associated with the experience of beauty. Other researchers proposed that the experience of beauty activated a network of connected brain regions instead of a single brain region; the network includes synchronized activity over the occipital, parietal, and frontal regions (Cela-Conde et al., 2013; Chatterjee & Vartanian, 2014; Vessel, Starr, & Rubin, 2012). That is, aesthetic experiences are dynamic states arising from the interactions between sensory-motor, emotion-valuation, and meaning-knowledges neural systems.Footnote 2

Furthermore, some researchers argued that different forms of beauty might be associated with common brain region(s), but the region(s) might not be specific to beauty. For example, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC, which includes mOFC and adjacent brain regions) is not only activated when experiencing beauty but also when processing hedonic values (Nadal et al., 2008; Pegors, Kable, Chatterjee, & Epstein, 2015; Skov, 2019; Skov & Nadal, 2018). Similarly, they proposed that the experience of beauty could not be distinguished from general pleasure (Nadal & Skov, 2018) and share the neural basis with other pleasure (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2015; Kringelbach & Berridge, 2009). Based on the qualitative comparison of meta-analyses from neuroaesthetics and reward processing, Skov (2019) argued that there may be the same underlying neural mechanisms for experiencing aesthetic objects and assessing values, such as liking for food and drinks. Although disagreeing on whether the vmPFC is beauty-specific, these researchers agree that the vmPFC is a common brain region associated with the experience of beauty.

In contrast to the evidence suggesting that the vmPFC/mOFC is the center of beauty in the brain, others reported different neural basis associated with the experience of beauty. For example, Silveira, Fehse, Vedder, Elvers, and Hennig-Fast (2015) found that paintings from the Museum of Modern Art elicited higher involvement of right precuneus, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and temporoparietal junction (TPJ), but not mOFC. Moreover, Brown, Gao, Tisdelle, Eickhoff, and Liotti (2011) conducted a meta-analysis focusing on an aesthetic appraisal. They examined the overlapping brain voxels associated with aesthetic appraisals from different modalities, including visual, audition, gustation, and olfaction. They found that a common neural basis might exist in the insula, a region typically associated with reward and emotion.

No conclusive findings in the field partially reflect that much evidence comes from theoretical analysis using studies with great variability in task and data analysis. Thus, the primary aim of the current study was to conduct a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of beauty to examine the cross-study convergence. The meta-analytical approach will overcome the common issues of low statistical power (Button et al., 2013) and high false-positive rates (Carp, 2012; Eklund, Nichols, & Knutsson, 2016; Wager, Lindquist, & Kaplan, 2007) in single neuroimaging studies by using the activation likelihood estimation (ALE) (Hu, Di, Li, Sui, & Peng, 2015; Wager et al., 2007). This approach is a well-developed coordinate-based meta-analysis technique which accommodates the spatial uncertainty of activation data in neuroimaging studies and allows researchers to form statistically defensible conclusions (Eickhoff et al., 2016; Fox, Lancaster, Laird, & Eickhoff, 2014; Laird et al., 2011). Note, we focused on the neuroimaging studies of the experience of visual beauty (Pearce et al., 2016). Specifically, we focused on the studies that compared the neural activities associated with the faces and visual arts (i.e., paintings, visual patterns, architectures, and dances) rated as beauty to those rated as nonbeauty. These two forms of beauty were selected because faces and visual arts represent two typical categories of beauty. Face beauty is the most representative natural beauty in social life, the preference of which is shaped by both evolution (Little, Jones, and DeBruine, 2011) and environment (Germine et al., 2015), whereas visual art is the most representative artificial beauty that reflects the subjective aesthetic preference of human beings (Skov & Nadal, 2020). Another methodological reason for this is that these lines of research could provide enough amount of studies for a meta-analysis with reasonable statistical power (Müller et al., 2018). A comparison of the ALE results of fMRI studies between these two types of beauty may provide reliable and meaningful results, which could serve as a valuable starting point for exploring a common neural representation of beauty.

Given that beauty is an abstract concept and difficult to define (Bergeron & Lopes, 2012; Conway & Rehding, 2013), the present meta-analysis used operational criteria for study selection. That is, we looked into the literature and checked whether a neuroimaging study not only used faces or visual art as stimuli but also reported the activities evoked by stimuli rated as beautiful (vs. not beautiful) or preferred (vs. not preferred). Specifically, fMRI studies reported the contrast between stimuli rated as beautiful versus stimuli rated as not beautiful, or studies in which the authors argued that the results contained the neural activities related to the subjective feeling of beauty in different tasks, such as aesthetic judgement, asymmetry judgment, or passive viewing. We assumed that a meta-analysis that synthesized these existing studies independent of task type could capture the common neural basis for the subjective feeling of beauty.

The goals of the current study were to (1) identify common brain regions activated by both the experience of visual art and face beauty, and (2) assess the specific brain regions activated by either the beauty of faces or the beauty of visual art. To fulfill these goals, we first conducted a systematic literature search of the neuroimaging studies on the beauty of visual art and face; we selected articles that included the brain data of experiencing beauty. Based on the data extracted from 49 selected neuroimaging studies, we conducted ALE meta-analyses for the beauty of visual art and the beauty of faces separately. Then, we applied conjunction and contrast analyses to the meta-analytical results to identify the common and distinct neural basis of the beauty of visual art and faces. In addition to this primary meta-analysis, we conducted another two sets of meta-analysis to assess the effect of heterogeneity of studies—an important issue in meta-analysis, one with a restrict criterion that only focused on the studies using a specific task (i.e., an explicit aesthetic judgment task), the other with a liberal criterion that included the studies regardless of stimulus domain and task type.

Methods

Literature search and study selection

Articles included in the present meta-analyses were identified based on a systematic literature search using specific terms in PubMed and the Web of Science (up to Dec 12, 2018). “Face” or “facial” were paired with “attractiveness,” “beauty” or “aesthetic” for aesthetic studies of faces; and “paintings,” “visual art,” “architecture,” or “dance” were paired with “beauty” or “aesthetic” for aesthetic studies of visual art. All terms were each combined (“AND”) with “functional magnetic resonance imaging or fMRI” or “positron emission tomography or PET” to identify relevant functional neuroimaging studies. For complete coverage, articles also were identified from recent meta-analyses and reviews (Boccia et al., 2016; Brown et al., 2011; Bzdok et al., 2011; Kirsch, Urgesi, & Cross, 2016; Mende-Siedlecki, Said, & Todorov, 2013; Vartanian & Skov, 2014). Additional studies were identified by searching through the reference lists of studies obtained via the initial search.

In our primary analysis, we selected studies based on the following inclusion and exclusion criteria:

  1. (1)

    Only studies reporting whole-brain analyses were included, whereas studies based on partial coverage or employing only region-of-interest analyses were excluded. One study was included after the author provided the whole-brain analyses with the contrast of current meta-analyses (Lebreton, Jorge, Michel, Thirion, & Pessiglione, 2009).

  2. (2)

    Articles reported results as coordinates in a standard reference frame (Talairach and Tournoux or MNI). To address problems induced by different coordinates used across the studies, we converted those coordinates published in the Talairach space to the MNI space using the Lancaster transformation (Lancaster et al., 2007).

  3. (3)

    Only studies with nonexpert young and middle-aged adults (18-50 years old) were included. Studies that included art experts were excluded if they did not report results for nonexperts separately (Kirk, Skov, Christensen & Nygaard, 2009) due to the influence of expertise on aesthetic appreciation (Hekkert & Wieringen, 1996).

  4. (4)

    Studies investigated the beauty appreciation of visual art or faces. Given that we are interested in the experience of beauty, we only included studies that reported the effect of beauty, i.e., the contrast between stimuli that was rated as beautiful versus stimuli that was rated as not beautiful. We further derived three subcriteria for this standard: 4a) studies using visual art or art-like stimuli or faces as stimuli; 4b) studies reporting the effect of beauty or the subjective preference for visual art or faces separately and directly, therefore, studies using visual art or faces as stimuli that did not report the effect of beauty or preference were excluded; also studies that did not report the effect of faces or visual art separately were excluded; and 4c) studies reported the effect of beauty by contrast the stimuli rated as beautiful to a high-level baseline (i.e., stimuli that have similar physical properties, e.g., luminance, visual complexity, but not rated as beautiful or not preferred by the participants), instead of low-level baselines (e.g., fixation or blank screen), so that the contrast included were specific to effect of beauty, instead of a combined effect of beauty and visual properties.

Coordinates of the peak activations of brain regions, which serves as our principal measures, were extracted from each article by searching the contrast of interest from both the main text and supplementary materials. For each experiment, only one contrast’s coordinate data were extracted. Beside the coordinates data, we also reported the number of valid participants, mean age or range of age, number of male participants, modality (fMRI or PET), stimuli, tasks, and the contrast from which the data were entered our meta-analyses (Table 1).

Table 1 Overview of the studies and contrasts included in the present meta-analyses

Data analysis

In addition to the primary analysis that used the above-mentioned four criteria, we also conducted two supplementary analyses. The first meta-analysis tested whether increasing the homogeneity of the included studies would result in better convergence among the included studies. To do this, a meta-analysis only included the studies using a specific task (i.e., an explicit aesthetic judgment task) from the primary meta-analysis. In contrast, another meta-analysis examined whether increasing the number of included studies would result in a better convergence because of greater statistical power. However, the liberal criterion also might increase the heterogeneity of studies for meta-analysis instead. If this is true, we would expect weaker results in this meta-analysis with liberal criteria compared with the primary analysis (see Supplementary Material). In this liberal meta-analysis, we selected studies without applying the subcriteria 4b and 4c.

Activation likelihood estimation

The revised ALE algorithm, which was implemented in Matlab code, for the coordinate-based meta-analysis of neuroimaging results (Eickhoff et al., 2009; Laird, Eickhoff, et al., 2009; Laird, Lancaster, & Fox, 2009; Turkeltaub, Eden, Jones, & Zeffiro, 2002). This algorithm was designed to identify areas that exhibit a convergence of reported coordinates across experiments that is higher than expected under a random spatial association. The key idea behind ALE is to treat the reported foci not as single points but rather as centers for 3D Gaussian probability distributions that capture the spatial uncertainty associated with each focus. The Full-Width Half-Maximum (FWHM) of these Gaussian functions was determined based on empirical data on the between-subject variance by the number of examined subjects per study, accommodating the notion that larger sample sizes should provide more reliable approximations of the “true” activation effect and, therefore, should be modeled by “smaller” Gaussian distributions (Eickhoff et al., 2009). Specifically, the number of subjects in the studies in our meta-analysis ranged from 6-87, with a median of 18, and the range of Full-Width Half-Maximum (FWHM) was from 8.5-10.94 mm (median: 9.5 mm).

The probabilities of all foci reported in a given experiment were then combined for each voxel, resulting in a modeled activation (MA) map (Turkeltaub et al., 2012). Taking the union across these MA maps yielded voxel-wise ALE scores that described the convergence of the results across experiments at each location of the brain. To distinguish ‘true’ convergence among studies from random convergence (i.e., noise), we compared ALE scores to an empirical null distribution reflecting a random spatial association among experiments. A random-effects inference was invoked, focusing on the inference on the above-chance convergence among studies rather than the clustering of foci within a particular study. Computationally, deriving this null-hypothesis involved sampling a voxel at random from each of the MA maps and taking the union of these values in the same manner as performed for the (spatially contingent) voxels in the true analysis, a process that can be solved analytically (Eickhoff, Bzdok, Laird, Kurth, & Fox, 2012). The p value of the “true” ALE was then given by the proportion of equal or higher values obtained under the null-distribution. The resulting nonparametric p values were then thresholded at the p < 0.05 (cluster-level corrected for multiple-comparison; cluster-forming threshold p < 0.001 at voxel level) (Eickhoff et al., 2012). All significant clusters were reported, and the volume, weighted center and locations, and Z-scores at the peaks within the regions are given.

Contrast and conjunction analysis of individual meta-analyses

To explore the distinct and common neural basis for two forms of beauty, we further conducted contrast and conjunction analyses based on the ALE results. Differences between conditions were tested by first performing separate ALE analyses for each condition and computing the voxel-wise difference between the ensuing ALE maps. All experiments contributing to either analysis were then pooled and randomly divided into two groups of the same size as the two original sets of experiments reflecting the contrasted ALE analyses (Eickhoff et al., 2011; Rottschy et al., 2012). The ALE scores for these two randomly assembled groups were calculated, and the differences between the ALE scores were recorded for each voxel in the brain. Repeating this process 25,000 times then yielded a null-distribution of differences in ALE scores between the two conditions. The “true” difference in the ALE scores was then tested against this voxel-wise null-distribution of label-exchangeability and thresholded at a probability of p > 95% for true differences. The conjunction analyses used the voxel-wise minimum of each single ALE results, i.e., finding the minimum z-value across the two thresholded ALE results voxel-wisely (Nichols, Brett, Andersson, Wager, & Poline, 2005).

Data Visualization

Given that there is no “gold standard” atlas for neuroimaging studies, we used probabilistic cytoarchitectonic maps (as implemented in SPM Anatomy Toolbox, the third version) (Amunts et al., 2013; Eickhoff, Heim, Zilles, & Amunts, 2006; Eickhoff et al., 2007; Eickhoff et al., 2005) to assign our resulting coordinates to anatomical structures. For visualization purposes, BrainNet Viewer (Xia, Wang, & He, 2013) was used.

Results

Studies included in the meta-analyses

Our primary analysis identified 49 articles: 20 articles for the beauty of visual arts, including 20 independent samples, 107 foci, and 295 subjects; 29 articles using attractive faces, including 29 independent sample, 183 foci, and 687 subjects. All of the code for search literature, endnote files for selecting articles, and metadata for the current study are available at https://osf.io/s9xds/. See Figure 1 for the process of article selection in detail and Table 1 for the information of selected articles.

Figure 1.
figure 1

Literature search for the beauty of visual art (A) and the beauty of faces (B), adapted from Liberati et al. (2009).

ALE Meta-analyses of the beauty of visual art and faces

Our primary analyses revealed that the frontal pole was convergently activated by the beauty of visual art. This brain region also was labeled as the anterior medial prefrontal cortex (aMPFC) in the literature (Table 2; Figure 2A). The ALE results of the beauty of faces showed that two brain regions were more convergently activated: the first region located in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) extending to the pgACC; the second region includes subcortical structures such as the ventral striatum and subcallosal cortex (Table 2; Figure 2B).

Table 2 Results of the meta-analyses for visual art and faces rated as beautiful
Figure 2.
figure 2

Results of the ALE meta-analysis and the contrast analysis. (A) Brain regions convergently activated more for visual art rated as beautiful than for visual art rated as not beautiful. (B) Brain regions convergently activated more for faces rated as beautiful than for faces rated as not beautiful. (C) The results of the contrast analysis between the ALE results of visual art rated as beautiful and faces rated as beautiful. Positive values (red) indicate greater activation for the beauty of visual art than for faces, and negative values (blue) indicate greater activation for the beauty of faces than for visual art.

Critically, the conjunction analysis found no survival cluster (see Supplementary Results for the additional exploration of the results). The contrast analysis further revealed that a locus within the pgACC and a locus within the left ventral striatum were more frequently activated by beautiful faces than by beautiful art, while there the left frontal pole was more activated by visual art rated as beautiful than by faces rated as beautiful (Table 3; Figure 2C).

Table 3 Contrast and conjunction analyses of the meta-analysis results

Meta-analyses with conservative inclusion standards

The first supplementary meta-analysis with more conservative inclusion standards (both explicit beauty judgment and beauty vs. nonbeauty contrast) resulted 11 studies for visual art (54 foci, 192 subject) and 16 studies for faces (97 foci, 383 subject). The analysis of the beauty of visual art did find any survival clusters. The analysis of beautiful faces resulted in one brain regions as in primary analysis: the pgACC (Table 4). These results suggested that there was not convergent activation for the experience of beauty across studies that used explicit beauty judgment task. Interestingly, meta-analysis with liberal inclusion standards (Supplementary Analysis 1) also did not show survived clusters for the beauty of visual art, but found similar results as in primary analysis for the beauty of faces: both vmPFC (extending to the pgACC) and the ventral striatum (extending to subcallosal cortex) were convergently activated (see Table S2 in Supplementary Materials).

Table 4 Results of the meta-analysis with conservative inclusive standards

Discussion

The present study was designed to test the convergent neural basis of experiencing the beauty of visual art and faces. The results did not reveal any overlapped brain regions linked to the two forms of visual beauty, but each of which was associated with distinct brain regions. The VMPFC/frontal pole was activated when experiencing visual art compared with face beauty rated, whereas the left ventral striatum and vmPFC/pgACC were associated with the experience of face beauty contrasting to visual art.

Is there a common neural basis for visual beauty?

Previous fMRI studies on beauty suggested the vmPFC/mOFC was involved in processing different forms of beauty (Ishizu & Zeki, 2011, 2013; Nadal et al., 2008; Pegors et al., 2015; Skov, 2019; Skov & Nadal, 2018; Zeki et al., 2014). It also has been reported that the vmPFC serves as a hub for subjective values (Etkin, Egner, Peraza, Kandel, & Hirsch, 2006; Levy & Glimcher, 2012; Roy, Shohamy, & Wager, 2012), positive signals (Bartra, McGuire, & Kable, 2013; Kalisch et al., 2006), and self-referential processes (Hu et al., 2016; Northoff et al., 2006). By synthesizing the available neuroimaging studies that examined the beauty of faces and visual art, the present meta-analysis did not find a convergent result between the neural activations elicited by these two forms of beauty. Moreover, this null result was robust even by increasing the homogeneity of the included studies. Given the strong statistical power of meta-analysis and its capacity to address the cross-study variance, the present results suggest that the common neural basis of beauty may not exist (also see Supplementary Materials).

One might argue that our primary meta-analysis was based on loose-defined operationalized criteria, which led to the high heterogeneity of the included studies (e.g., studies using different tasks). The heterogeneity resulted in the null result of the conjunction. However, the argument was not supported by the meta-analysis using the conservative inclusive criteria, which failed to show convergent results. Although the additional meta-analysis, which had a small number of studies, might suffer from low statistical power, in contrast to the convergent results in the primary analysis, it suggests that heterogeneity was less important than the number of studies in the current analysis. These results indicated that the primary analysis, with a relatively large number of studies, at least detected some shared mental processes associated with the experience of beauty. Otherwise, if the studies with different tasks evoked different mental processes (i.e., increased heterogeneity), the primary meta-analysis should have found less convergent results than the meta-analysis with conservative criteria.

Understanding a common mechanism of experience beauty is of great interest not only in empirical aesthetics but also in aesthetics in general because of the long-last debate about the center of beauty. Previous theories in neuroaesthetics suggest a common neural basis representing beauty in the brain (Ishizu & Zeki, 2011, 2013; Nadal et al., 2008; Pegors et al., 2015; Skov, 2019; Skov & Nadal, 2018; Zeki et al., 2014). However, this view was not supported by the present meta-analyses.

The discrepancy between the strong theoretical prediction of a common neural basis for beauty and lack of empirical evidence calls for more rigorous studies. Recently, the field of cognitive neuroscience has been challenged for its reproducibility (Botvinik-Nezer et al., 2020; Hu, Jiang, Jeffrey, & Zuo, 2018; Poldrack et al., 2017). Small sample size (which results in low statistical power) (Button et al., 2013), flexibility in data analysis (Botvinik-Nezer et al., 2020; Carp, 2012), and errors in implementing software (Eklund et al., 2016), accompanied by publication bias (Jennings & Horn, 2012), all threatened the reliability and reproducibility of the field (Hu et al., 2018; Poldrack et al., 2017). Direct replication, albeit very few, found that pessimistic results (Boekel et al., 2015). To better test the common neural basis of beauty, researchers need to adopt new practices that have been recommended recently, including the standard reporting, preregistration, and large sample size (Munafò et al., 2017). Also, as science is accumulative, further studies can be integrated into the current meta-analysis to update the results. To facilitate this process, we have opened the meta-data of this meta-analysis (see Method section). Future works can easily be integrated to update the knowledge about the common neural basis of beauty.

Unique neural basis underlying the beauty of faces and visual art

The ALE results of face beauty demonstrated that experiencing face beauty induced greater activation in the vmPFC/pgACC and the left ventral striatum than when faces rated as not beautiful. In contrast, the ALE results of the beauty of visual art showed convergent activations in the left frontal pole/aMPFC related to visual art rated as not beautiful. The result was consistent with previous studies demonstrating that the vmPFC-subcortical rewarding system is engaged in processing the beauty of faces (Chatterjee & Vartanian, 2014; Hahn & Perrett, 2014). On the other hand, the vmPFC/pgACC and the ventral striatum play different roles in cognition. For example, the vmPFC/pgACC is activated with a wide range of brain structures when involved in multiple higher-level functions (de la Vega, Chang, Banich, Wager, & Yarkoni, 2016; Roy et al., 2012). It has consistently been reported that the ventral striatum is engaged in reward processing (Haber & Knutson, 2010; Liu, Hairston, Schrier, & Fan, 2011), especially primary reward (Sescousse, Caldú, Segura, & Dreher, 2013). Therefore, it is possible the facial beauty is appreciated through a ventral pathway: the ventral striatum primarily responds to the rewarding value of faces, the reward signal, along with other information, and then integrated into the vmPFC to generate positive affections.

In contrast, the frontal pole/aMPFC convergently activated in appreciating the beauty of visual art is more engaged in high-level, top-down processing. Previous studies have shown that it involves in episodic memory, decision-making, and social cognition (de la Vega et al., 2016), such as positive evaluation (Bartra et al., 2013) and secondary reward (Sescousse et al., 2013). We speculated that the aMPFC, when appreciating visual art, links more abstract beauty to reward, like that of secondary reward.

Role of the sensory cortex and hemispheric differences in processing beauty

We did not find activations in the sensory regions in meta-analyses of either the beauty of faces or the beauty of visual art. For the facial beauty, no significant activations in either the fusiform face areas or other sensory cortical areas were observed when rating faces as beautiful contrasting to rating faces as not beautiful. Likewise, there was no activation of the sensory-motor network observed when experiencing the beauty of visual art. These results seem to contradict previous theories about facial beauty (Chatterjee et al., 2009; Iaria et al., 2008) and art appreciating (Boccia et al., 2016; Chatterjee & Vartanian, 2014; Leder & Nadal, 2014).

One possibility of these results is that current meta-analyses exclusively included the contrasts between beautiful versus non-beautiful stimuli with similar perceptual features (high-level baselines, see Method section), therefore, eliminating the effect of the physical feature processing on the experience of visual beauty. If this is the case, the present results indicated that the beauty of faces and visual arts showed different neural representations at high-level processing.

Notably, our results do not suggest that physical features are less important in beauty appreciating. The sensory network is necessary for processing the beauty of faces and visual art (Chatterjee & Vartanian, 2014). The shared standard for faces rated as beautiful (Hönekopp, 2006; Leder, Goller, Rigotti, & Forster, 2016) suggests that physical features are crucial for experiencing visual beauty. Hence, future studies are needed to examine the contribution of sensory processing to beauty appreciation.

Regarding the hemisphere differences, it seems that peak locations in experiencing both face and art beauty appear in the left hemisphere, contradicting the view that the right brain is dominant for imagination, creativity, and emotions (Bromberger et al., 2011). However, our meta-analytic results did not reveal hemispheric asymmetry for experiencing beauty: most clusters were near the midline of the brain. The present results, together with the previous meta-analysis on appreciating visual art (Boccia et al., 2016), suggest that the visual beauty is processed by both hemispheres.

Further considerations

Based on the available research in the literature, the current meta-analysis attempted to examine whether there is a common beauty of faces and visual art in the brain and whether albeit each category of beauty had its convergent brain regions. One limitation of the current meta-analysis is that the analyses were based on the reported peak activations, which may have two disadvantages. First, a large part of the spatial information from the original study was discarded. However, this limitation can be alleviated by the fact that the results derived from image-based meta-analysis are in good agreement with coordinate-based meta-analysis approaches (Salimi-Khorshidi, Smith, Keltner, Wager, & Nichols, 2009). Second, studies that did not report any activations for the contrast of interest were not included in the current analyses. Indeed, some studies used the beauty appreciation task but did not report the results of beautiful-non-beautiful contrast (Kampe, Frith, Dolan, & Frith, 2001); therefore, they could not be included in our meta-analysis. However, this selection bias, if any, together with potential publication bias in neuroimaging studies (Jennings & Horn, 2012), may result in inflated false positive. This inflated false-positive rate is not a concern for the current meta-analysis, because no positive results for the conjunction analysis were observed in the present study. A related issue is that coordinate-based, meta-analysis approaches of neuroimaging studies, such as ALE, use the averaged likelihood in common volumetric space (Wager et al., 2007); it might lead to false positives of convergent activation in adjacent regions across studies. However, this is not a concern in the current meta-analysis given our negative results.

The meta-analyses inherently include many seems trivial but important choices that bring extra flexibility in research practices. For example, the standard for inclusion may not be clear for a few papers, and results from more than one contrast are qualified with the inclusion standard. In these cases, the final choice might be arbitrary. Studies have shown that flexibility in research practice can inflate the false-positive rate (Botvinik-Nezer et al., 2020; Carp, 2012; Simmons, Nelson, & Simonsohn, 2011). It also might be true for meta-analysis. To alleviate this limitation (Schönbrodt, Maier, Heene, & Zehetleitner, 2015), we adopted an open and transparent practice and uploaded the article selection process (as recorded in Endnote® X8, Clarivate Analytics, Philadelphia, PA) and meta-data (see https://osf.io/s9xds/).

Conclusions

Our meta-analytic results revealed distinct neural specificities for visual art and face beauty but lacked evidence for the common neural basis of visual beauty. This null result suggests that the available data did not support the notion of the existence of a common brain for processing different forms of beauty. To support such a common neural basis for beauty, more rigorous studies are needed.